{ "paper_id": "2021", "header": { "generated_with": "S2ORC 1.0.0", "date_generated": "2023-01-19T12:16:32.730196Z" }, "title": "Graph-theoretic Properties of the Class of Phonological Neighbourhood Networks", "authors": [ { "first": "Rory", "middle": [], "last": "Turnbull", "suffix": "", "affiliation": { "laboratory": "", "institution": "Newcastle University", "location": {} }, "email": "rory.turnbull@newcastle.ac.uk" } ], "year": "", "venue": null, "identifiers": {}, "abstract": "This paper concerns the structure of phonological neighbourhood networks, which are a graph-theoretic representation of the phonological lexicon. These networks represent", "pdf_parse": { "paper_id": "2021", "_pdf_hash": "", "abstract": [ { "text": "This paper concerns the structure of phonological neighbourhood networks, which are a graph-theoretic representation of the phonological lexicon. These networks represent", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Abstract", "sec_num": null } ], "body_text": [ { "text": "each word as a node and links are placed between words which are phonological neighbours, usually defined as a string edit distance of one. Phonological neighbourhood networks have been used to study many aspects of the mental lexicon and psycholinguistic theories of speech production and perception. This paper offers preliminary graph-theoretic observations about phonological neighbourhood networks considered as a class. To aid this exploration, this paper introduces the concept of the hyperlexicon, the network consisting of all possible words for a given symbol set and their neighbourhood relations. The construction of the hyperlexicon is discussed, and basic properties are derived. This work is among the first to directly address the nature of phonological neighbourhood networks from an analytic perspective.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "", "sec_num": null }, { "text": "Recent work in phonological psycholinguistics has investigated the structure of the lexicon through the use of phonological neighbourhood networks (Chan and Vitevitch, 2010; Turnbull and Peperkamp, 2017; Siew, 2013; Siew and Vitevitch, 2020; Shoemark et al., 2016) . A phonological neighbourhood network is a representation of the lexicon where each word is treated as a node and a link is placed between nodes if and only if those two nodes are phonological neighbours. Two words are neighbours if their string edit distance, in terms of phonological representation, is one. In other words, the neighbours of a word w are all the words that can be formed by the addition, deletion, or substitution of a single phoneme from w. The neighbourhood relation is symmetric (if w is a neighbour of w , then w is necessarily a neighbour of w), intransitive (if w is a neighbour of w , and w is a neighbour of w , it is not necessarily the case that w is a neighbour of w ), and anti-reflexive (w cannot be a neighbour of itself). Figure 1 shows an abbreviated phonological neighbourhood network for some words of English. One advantage of this representation is that it permits analysis with the methods of network science and graph theory, and work so far has shown a good deal of promise in modeling psycholinguistic properties of the lexicon with these methods (Chan and Vitevitch, 2010; Vitevitch, 2008) . A common analysis technique within network science is to compare a given network with a randomly generated one that has the same number of nodes and links. Notable features of the target network relative to the random network are likely due to intrinsic properties of the target network, rather than chance. From this structure one can then infer details about the organising principles that generated the network originally.", "cite_spans": [ { "start": 147, "end": 173, "text": "(Chan and Vitevitch, 2010;", "ref_id": "BIBREF1" }, { "start": 174, "end": 203, "text": "Turnbull and Peperkamp, 2017;", "ref_id": "BIBREF12" }, { "start": 204, "end": 215, "text": "Siew, 2013;", "ref_id": "BIBREF8" }, { "start": 216, "end": 241, "text": "Siew and Vitevitch, 2020;", "ref_id": "BIBREF9" }, { "start": 242, "end": 264, "text": "Shoemark et al., 2016)", "ref_id": "BIBREF7" }, { "start": 1356, "end": 1382, "text": "(Chan and Vitevitch, 2010;", "ref_id": "BIBREF1" }, { "start": 1383, "end": 1399, "text": "Vitevitch, 2008)", "ref_id": "BIBREF13" } ], "ref_spans": [ { "start": 1022, "end": 1030, "text": "Figure 1", "ref_id": "FIGREF0" } ], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Motivation", "sec_num": "1" }, { "text": "For phonological neighbourhood networks, however, this method is often inappropriate, as many logically possible network structures are not possible phonological neighbourhood networks. This fact is because the links between nodes-the neighbourhood relations-are intrinsic to the definitions of the nodes themselves. Changing a link between nodes necessarily means changing the content of a node, which then could entail other changes to other links. This problem was highlighted by Turnbull and Peperkamp (2017), 1 who instead chose to randomly generate lexicons and derive networks from those lexicons. However, randomly generated lexicons do not guarantee the same number of links will be present in the resulting network, making it difficult to compare like with like. For this reason, studying phonological neighbourhood networks as a class, and discovering their defining characteristics, is an important methodological goal for psycholinguists.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Motivation", "sec_num": "1" }, { "text": "This research therefore seeks to answer the following broad questions: What are the distinctive characteristics of phonological neighbourhood networks, including their definitions in terms of edge sets and vertex sets, their extremal properties, and characterization of forbidden subgraphs? Is there an effective and efficient method by which phonological neighbourhood graphs can be distinguished from other graphs? The present paper lays the mathematical foundations for future investigations of both of these questions.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Motivation", "sec_num": "1" }, { "text": "This section briefly defines the basic mathematical definitions and operations used in the remainder of the paper. The reader is referred to standard textbooks in graph theory, such as Trudeau (1993) or Diestel (2005) , for more details. As mathematical terminology and notation can vary between subfields, alternative names and characterizations of some objects are mentioned in the ensuing sections, but they are not strictly necessary to understand the arguments of this paper.", "cite_spans": [ { "start": 185, "end": 199, "text": "Trudeau (1993)", "ref_id": "BIBREF11" }, { "start": 203, "end": 217, "text": "Diestel (2005)", "ref_id": "BIBREF2" } ], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Preliminaries", "sec_num": "2" }, { "text": "Networks can be modeled as mathematical objects known as graphs, which consist of vertices (nodes) and edges (links). Let G be an undirected graph with no self-loops with vertex set V (G) and edge set E(G). Let K n denote the complete graph with n vertices and all possible edges.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Preliminaries", "sec_num": "2" }, { "text": "A graph H is said to be a subgraph of a graph", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Preliminaries", "sec_num": "2" }, { "text": "G if V (H) \u2286 V (G) and E(H) \u2286 V (G), that is,", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Preliminaries", "sec_num": "2" }, { "text": "if the the edges and vertices of H are subsets of those of G. A subgraph H is an induced subgraph of G if every edge in E(G) whose endpoints are both in V (H) is present in E(H). In other words, an induced subgraph can be obtained by the process of removing vertices (and any incident edges) from a graph, but not removing edges on their own. Figure 2 provides illustrative examples.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [ { "start": 343, "end": 351, "text": "Figure 2", "ref_id": null } ], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Preliminaries", "sec_num": "2" }, { "text": "The diamond is K 4 with one edge removed. A circle C k has the set of nodes {1, 2, ..., k} and edge set {{1, 2}, {2, 3}, ..., {(k \u2212 1), k}, {k, 1}}. (Circle graphs that are induced subgraphs of a larger graph are also known as k-holes.) Figure 3 depicts the diamond and C 5 .", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [ { "start": 237, "end": 245, "text": "Figure 3", "ref_id": null } ], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Preliminaries", "sec_num": "2" }, { "text": "A star S k is a graph with one central vertex which is connected to k other unique vertices. No other vertices or edges exist. Figure 4 depicts the stars S 3 (also known as a claw), S 4 , and S 6 .", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [ { "start": 127, "end": 135, "text": "Figure 4", "ref_id": null } ], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Preliminaries", "sec_num": "2" }, { "text": "The Cartesian product A \u00d7 B of two sets A and B is defined as", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Preliminaries", "sec_num": "2" }, { "text": "EQUATION", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [ { "start": 0, "end": 8, "text": "EQUATION", "ref_id": "EQREF", "raw_str": "A \u00d7 B = {(a, b)|a \u2208 A, b \u2208 B},", "eq_num": "(1)" } ], "section": "Preliminaries", "sec_num": "2" }, { "text": "that is, the Cartesian product of A and B is the set of all ordered pairs where the first element is a member of A and the second element is a member of B. For example, the Cartesian product of {a, b, c} and {x, y} is", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Preliminaries", "sec_num": "2" }, { "text": "{(a, x), (a, y), (b, x), (b, y), (c, x), (c, y)}.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Preliminaries", "sec_num": "2" }, { "text": "The Cartesian product G H of two graphs G and H has the vertex set", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Preliminaries", "sec_num": "2" }, { "text": "EQUATION", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [ { "start": 0, "end": 8, "text": "EQUATION", "ref_id": "EQREF", "raw_str": "V (G H) = V (G) \u00d7 V (H).", "eq_num": "(2)" } ], "section": "Preliminaries", "sec_num": "2" }, { "text": "A given vertex (a, x) is linked with another vertex (b, y) if a = b (the first elements are identical) and {x, y} \u2208 E(H) (the second elements are linked in H), or if x = y (the second elements are identical) and {a, b} \u2208 E(G) (the first elements are linked in G). To aid understanding, Figure 5 depicts an example of the Cartesian product of two graphs, G and H. Graph G has V (G) = {a, b, c} and E(G) = {{a, b}, {b, c}}. Graph H has V (H) = {x, y} and E(H) = {{x, y}}. Observe how G and H can be seen in G H as two orthogonal dimensions. Note also that the total number of vertices in G H is equal to the product of the number of vertices in G and H. We further denote the Cartesian exponent of a graph G as that is, the Cartesian product of G with itself n \u2212 1 times. Figure 6 depicts the graphs K 2 and K 3 2 . It can be seen that K 2 2 is a square, K 3 2 is a cube, and K n 2 is an n-dimensional hypercube. 2 Likewise, the vertex labels of K n m are equivalent to all strings of length n drawn from an alphabet of m symbols. The edges of K n m are equivalent to the neighbourhood relations of such strings. These facts establish the basis upon which we can use these tools to model phonological neighbourhood networks.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [ { "start": 286, "end": 294, "text": "Figure 5", "ref_id": null }, { "start": 770, "end": 778, "text": "Figure 6", "ref_id": "FIGREF2" } ], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Preliminaries", "sec_num": "2" }, { "text": "EQUATION", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [ { "start": 0, "end": 8, "text": "EQUATION", "ref_id": "EQREF", "raw_str": "G n = G G G . . . G n ,", "eq_num": "(3)" } ], "section": "Preliminaries", "sec_num": "2" }, { "text": "((1, 0), 0) ((1, 1), 0) ((0, 1), 0) K 2 K 3 2 ((0, 0), 1) ((1, 0), 1) ((1, 1), 1) ((0, 1), 1)", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Preliminaries", "sec_num": "2" }, { "text": "In this paper we introduce the concept of the hyperlexicon. A hyperlexicon H(\u03c6, L) is defined as the phonological neighbourhood network generated from all possible string sequences of lengths { 1 , . . . n } for \u2208 L over an alphabet of length \u03c6. Figure 7 depicts the hyperlexicon of all 'words' of length 1, 2, and 3, over the alphabet of 0 and 1. Stella and Brede (2015) observed that the set of all possible phoneme sequences (i.e. the hyperlexicon) is composed of multiple 'layers', each corresponding to a distinct member of L. This layered struc-ture can be clearly seen in Figure 7 . Edges within a layer correspond to neighbours by substitution, while edges between layers correspond to neighbours by deletion or insertion. Note further that each layer is isomorphic to K \u03c6 , the th Cartesian exponent of the complete graph with \u03c6 vertices. 3 Imagine now a hypothetical lexicon consisting of the words 1, 00, 10, and 110. The phonological neighbourhood network of this lexicon is depicted in Figure 8 , overlaid on the hyperlexicon from Figure 7. It can be seen that this lexicon's network is an induced subgraph of the hyperlexicon.", "cite_spans": [ { "start": 348, "end": 371, "text": "Stella and Brede (2015)", "ref_id": "BIBREF10" }, { "start": 848, "end": 849, "text": "3", "ref_id": null } ], "ref_spans": [ { "start": 246, "end": 254, "text": "Figure 7", "ref_id": null }, { "start": 579, "end": 587, "text": "Figure 7", "ref_id": null }, { "start": 999, "end": 1007, "text": "Figure 8", "ref_id": "FIGREF3" } ], "eq_spans": [], "section": "The Hyperlexicon", "sec_num": "3" }, { "text": "Indeed, phonological neighbourhood networks are necessarily induced subgraphs of the hyperlexicon. For example, the English lexicon consists of strings of varying lengths, with the set of English phonemes as its 'alphabet'. There are some strings of English phonemes which are not part of the English lexicon-i.e. nonwords such as blick and pmisgkr. The set of words in the English lexicon, then, is a subset of the set of all logically possible strings of English phonemes. A hyperlexicon corresponds to the neighbourhood network derived from a set of all logically possible strings of length L, given some set of phonemes of length \u03c6. Any subset of this set of strings will correspond to an induced subgraph of the hyperlexicon. Any phonological neighbourhood network, then, with words of lengths in L constituted from \u03c6 distinct phonemes, is necessarily an induced subgraph of the hyperlexicon H(\u03c6, L). Studying properties of the hyperlexicon therefore gives us insight into the possible structures of phonological neighbourhood networks.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "The Hyperlexicon", "sec_num": "3" }, { "text": "The vertex set of the hyperlexicon is given by", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "The Hyperlexicon", "sec_num": "3" }, { "text": "EQUATION", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [ { "start": 0, "end": 8, "text": "EQUATION", "ref_id": "EQREF", "raw_str": "V (H(\u03c6, L)) = \u2208L V (K \u03c6 ),", "eq_num": "(4)" } ], "section": "The Hyperlexicon", "sec_num": "3" }, { "text": "that is, the set union of each layer's vertices. The number of vertices of H(\u03c6, L) is the sum of the size of each layer, which is", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "The Hyperlexicon", "sec_num": "3" }, { "text": "EQUATION", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [ { "start": 0, "end": 8, "text": "EQUATION", "ref_id": "EQREF", "raw_str": "L \u03c6 .", "eq_num": "(5)" } ], "section": "The Hyperlexicon", "sec_num": "3" }, { "text": "If L is contiguous, H(\u03c6, L) is necessarily connected (i.e. there is exactly one connected component); if L is not contiguous, 4 then H(\u03c6, L) has multiple connected components.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "The Hyperlexicon", "sec_num": "3" }, { "text": "Defining the edge set of H(\u03c6, L) is less straightforward than the vertex set and is not fully solved. Within each layer, the edges are the same as in the graph K \u03c6 . Between the layers the situation is considerably more complex. To begin, we first determine the number of possible unique neighbours for any word. For a word of length in a language 4 Such a scenario is plausible for languages with strict phonotactics requiring an obligatory onset and forbidding codas, i.e. all syllables must be CV. For such languages, L = {2, 4, 6, 8, . . . }. Hua (Blevins, 1995) and Senufo (Kientz, 1979) have been reported to have this kind of syllable structure.", "cite_spans": [ { "start": 551, "end": 566, "text": "(Blevins, 1995)", "ref_id": "BIBREF0" }, { "start": 578, "end": 592, "text": "(Kientz, 1979)", "ref_id": "BIBREF5" } ], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "The Edges between the Layers of the Hyperlexicon", "sec_num": "4" }, { "text": "with \u03c6 distinct phonemes, neighbours are generated through the addition, deletion, or substitution of a single phoneme. The number of possible neighbours can be shown to depend upon word length , alphabet size \u03c6, and the number of pairs of adjacent identical phonemes, described below.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "The Edges between the Layers of the Hyperlexicon", "sec_num": "4" }, { "text": "It is straightforward to demonstrate that there are \u03c6 \u2212", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Substitutions", "sec_num": "4.1" }, { "text": "possible substitutions. This statement follows from the fact that neighbourhood is an anti-reflexive relation, so vacuously substituting a phoneme for itself will not generate a neighbour.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Substitutions", "sec_num": "4.1" }, { "text": "The number of additions can be derived from the fact that each of \u03c6 symbols can be added to + 1 positions, which gives \u03c6( + 1). However, for each insertion position, one of these \u03c6 phonemes will result in a string which is identical to an insertion of the same phoneme at a different location. For example, prefixing a onto the beginning of ab is equivalent to inserting a into the middle of ab: they both result in aab. The number of additions is therefore \u03c6( + 1) \u2212", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Additions", "sec_num": "4.2" }, { "text": "which simplifies to Equation (6) plus \u03c6:", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Additions", "sec_num": "4.2" }, { "text": "EQUATION", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [ { "start": 0, "end": 8, "text": "EQUATION", "ref_id": "EQREF", "raw_str": "\u03c6 \u2212 + \u03c6.", "eq_num": "(8)" } ], "section": "Additions", "sec_num": "4.2" }, { "text": "The number of deletions is not constant and depends upon the structure of the word. For example, although there are three distinct deletion positions in a possible word aaa, all three of them lead to the same unique word aa; so practically speaking", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Deletions", "sec_num": "4.3" }, { "text": "K 5 C 4", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Deletions", "sec_num": "4.3" }, { "text": "Figure 9: In attempting to generate C 4 (right) from K 5 (left), a single vertex must be removed. However, as shown by the middle graph, removal of a single vertex (in grey) results in a graph with too many edges. This is true no matter which vertex we choose to remove. C 4 is therefore not an induced subgraph of K 5 ; we can call C 4 a forbidden subgraph of K 5 .", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Deletions", "sec_num": "4.3" }, { "text": "there is only one possible deletion. On the other hand, all three possible deletions on abc result in three unique strings (namely, bc, ac, ab), so it has three deletions. The actual number of possible deletions depends on the number of pairs of identical adjacent symbols. 5 Pairs of identical adjacent symbols act as a single symbol for the purposes of counting possible deletion sites. The number of deletions is therefore \u2212 a,", "cite_spans": [ { "start": 274, "end": 275, "text": "5", "ref_id": null } ], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Deletions", "sec_num": "4.3" }, { "text": "where a is the number of pairs of adjacent identical symbols in the word. For example abba has only 3 possible deletions, despite being of length 4. (For this word, a deletion at position 2 is equivalent to a deletion at position 3; the sequence bb can be essentially treated as a single symbol for the purposes of counting deletion sites.) All words allow at least 1 deletion, and some words allow as many as deletions.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Deletions", "sec_num": "4.3" }, { "text": "From the sections above, it follows that each vertex in H(\u03c6, L) has \u03c6 \u2212 edges to other nodes in the same layer as it. If there is a higher layer, then each node also has \u03c6 \u2212 + \u03c6 edges leading to nodes in that layer. If there is a lower layer, then each node has between 1 and edges leading to that layer.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Vertex Degree", "sec_num": "4.4" }, { "text": "Finally, we begin to attempt to characterize the class of hyperlexicons in terms of forbidden subgraphs. A forbidden subgraph of G is any graph which is not isomorphic to any induced subgraph of G. For example, there is no induced subgraph of K 5 which is isomorphic to C 4 . This fact is illustrated in Figure 9 . C 4 is therefore a forbidden subgraph of K 5 . Graph structures which are impossible within a hyperlexicon are also impossible within real phonological networks, because real phonological networks are induced subgraphs of a hyperlexicon. Understanding the forbidden subgraphs of a hyperlexicon therefore allows us to understand possible natural language networks.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [ { "start": 304, "end": 312, "text": "Figure 9", "ref_id": null } ], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Forbidden Subgraphs", "sec_num": "5" }, { "text": "A hyperlexicon is composed of layers. Each layer is K \u03c6 , the th cartesian exponent of K \u03c6 . For the special case of = 2 (i.e. words of length two), these graphs have been studied in the mathematical literature under the names of Rooks' graphs, gridline graphs, adjacency graphs, and graphs of (0, 1) matrices. Peterson (2003) studied these graphs in cases where > 2, and established that the diamond and C 5 are among the forbidden subgraphs of K \u03c6 .", "cite_spans": [ { "start": 311, "end": 326, "text": "Peterson (2003)", "ref_id": "BIBREF6" } ], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Forbidden Subgraphs of individual layers", "sec_num": "5.1" }, { "text": "The 3-star S 3 has been shown to be a forbidden subgraph of K 2 \u03c6 (Hedetniemi, 1971) . More generally, no layer at length has S +1 as an induced subgraph. This observation follows from the pigeonhole principle: the first vertices of S +1 can be found in the dimensions of the graph. The final vertex must be in one of the dimensions already considered, and therefore must be adjacent to an existing vertex. This leads to a triangle, meaning the induced subgraph is no longer a star.", "cite_spans": [ { "start": 66, "end": 84, "text": "(Hedetniemi, 1971)", "ref_id": "BIBREF4" } ], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Forbidden Subgraphs of individual layers", "sec_num": "5.1" }, { "text": "These structures, forbidden from each individual layer of the hyperlexicon, are not forbidden from the hyperlexicon as a whole. Within the hyperlexicon H(3, {1, 2, 3}) we observe both the diamond and C 5 ; see Figures 10 and 11. 6 Similarly, for a hy- 6 We have been unable to find any induced C5 in cases where \u03c6 < 3. While this conjecture might be of mathematical interest, it is not relevant to our main use-case of phonological perlexicon with max(L) = 4, the star S 5 is present as an induced subgraph, as shown in Figure 12 . Since these structures cannot occur within each layer, it follows that their existence within a hyperlexicon must necessarily span more than one layer. Indeed, we hypothesize that in the case of S +1 where = max(L), this structure must necessarily span three layers.", "cite_spans": [ { "start": 210, "end": 230, "text": "Figures 10 and 11. 6", "ref_id": null }, { "start": 252, "end": 253, "text": "6", "ref_id": null } ], "ref_spans": [ { "start": 520, "end": 529, "text": "Figure 12", "ref_id": "FIGREF0" } ], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Forbidden Subgraphs of individual layers", "sec_num": "5.1" }, { "text": "No hyperlexicon has K \u03c6+2 as an induced subgraph. K \u03c6 exists, as this constitutes the 'dimensions' of each layer. From K \u03c6 it is possible to induce K \u03c6+1 by adding a vertex from one layer down. For example, the string a is adjacent to aa, ab, ac, and so on. However there is no other vertex in the lower layer which is adjacent to all of a's neighbours and to a itself. K \u03c6+2 is therefore not an induced subgraph of the hyperlexicon.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Forbidden Subgraphs of the Entire Hyperlexicon", "sec_num": "5.2" }, { "text": "This paper has reviewed the basic structure of hyperlexicon graphs. Induced subgraphs of hyperlexicon graphs typify the class of phonological neighbourhood networks. It is hoped that the preliminary results presented here will spur further work on the networks, as all known natural languages possess considerably more than 3 phonemes. nature of phonological neighbourhood networks as formal objects. This work in turn has methodological implications for evaluating and measuring phonological neighbourhood networks derived from natural languages.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Conclusion", "sec_num": "6" }, { "text": "See also Gruenenfelder and Pisoni (2009) for related concerns.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "", "sec_num": null }, { "text": "More generally, K n m is an m \u00d7 m Rook's graph in n dimensions.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "", "sec_num": null }, { "text": "Each layer can also be characterized as an expansion of the hypercube graph Q , or as a Hamming graph Ham( , \u03c6).", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "", "sec_num": null }, { "text": "Using the terminology of combinatorics on words, a \"pair of identical adjacent symbols\" can be understood as a square of length 2.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "", "sec_num": null } ], "back_matter": [ { "text": "I am grateful to Paul Tupper and several anonymous reviewers for their comments on an earlier draft. All errors and omissions are my own.", "cite_spans": [], "ref_spans": [], "eq_spans": [], "section": "Acknowledgements", "sec_num": null } ], "bib_entries": { "BIBREF0": { "ref_id": "b0", "title": "The syllable in phonological theory", "authors": [ { "first": "Juliette", "middle": [], "last": "Blevins", "suffix": "" } ], "year": 1995, "venue": "Handbook of phonological theory", "volume": "", "issue": "", "pages": "206--244", "other_ids": {}, "num": null, "urls": [], "raw_text": "Juliette Blevins. 1995. The syllable in phonological theory. In John Goldsmith, editor, Handbook of phonological theory, pages 206-244. Blackwell.", "links": null }, "BIBREF1": { "ref_id": "b1", "title": "Network structure influences speech production", "authors": [ { "first": "K Y", "middle": [], "last": "Chan", "suffix": "" }, { "first": "M S", "middle": [], "last": "Vitevitch", "suffix": "" } ], "year": 2010, "venue": "Cognitive Science", "volume": "34", "issue": "4", "pages": "685--697", "other_ids": { "DOI": [ "10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01100.x" ] }, "num": null, "urls": [], "raw_text": "K Y Chan and M S Vitevitch. 2010. Network struc- ture influences speech production. Cognitive Sci- ence, 34(4):685-697.", "links": null }, "BIBREF2": { "ref_id": "b2", "title": "Graph Theory", "authors": [ { "first": "Reinhard", "middle": [], "last": "Diestel", "suffix": "" } ], "year": 2005, "venue": "", "volume": "", "issue": "", "pages": "", "other_ids": {}, "num": null, "urls": [], "raw_text": "Reinhard Diestel. 2005. Graph Theory, 3rd edition. Springer, New York, NY.", "links": null }, "BIBREF3": { "ref_id": "b3", "title": "The lexical restructuring hypothesis and graph theoretic analyses of networks based on random lexicons", "authors": [ { "first": "M", "middle": [], "last": "Thomas", "suffix": "" }, { "first": "David", "middle": [ "B" ], "last": "Gruenenfelder", "suffix": "" }, { "first": "", "middle": [], "last": "Pisoni", "suffix": "" } ], "year": 2009, "venue": "Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research", "volume": "52", "issue": "2", "pages": "596--609", "other_ids": { "DOI": [ "10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0004)" ] }, "num": null, "urls": [], "raw_text": "Thomas M Gruenenfelder and David B Pisoni. 2009. The lexical restructuring hypothesis and graph the- oretic analyses of networks based on random lexi- cons. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(2):596-609.", "links": null }, "BIBREF4": { "ref_id": "b4", "title": "Graphs of (0,1)-matrices", "authors": [ { "first": "; M", "middle": [], "last": "Stephen T Hedetniemi", "suffix": "" }, { "first": "J", "middle": [], "last": "Capobianco", "suffix": "" }, { "first": "M", "middle": [], "last": "B Frechen", "suffix": "" }, { "first": "", "middle": [], "last": "Krolik", "suffix": "" } ], "year": 1971, "venue": "Recent Trends in Graph Theory", "volume": "", "issue": "", "pages": "157--171", "other_ids": {}, "num": null, "urls": [], "raw_text": "Stephen T Hedetniemi. 1971. Graphs of (0,1)-matrices. In M Capobianco, J B Frechen, and M Krolik, ed- itors, Recent Trends in Graph Theory, pages 157- 171. Springer, Berlin.", "links": null }, "BIBREF5": { "ref_id": "b5", "title": "Dieu et les g\u00e9nies: R\u00e9cits \u00e9tiologiques senoufo", "authors": [ { "first": "Albert", "middle": [], "last": "Kientz", "suffix": "" } ], "year": 1979, "venue": "", "volume": "", "issue": "", "pages": "", "other_ids": {}, "num": null, "urls": [], "raw_text": "Albert Kientz. 1979. Dieu et les g\u00e9nies: R\u00e9cits \u00e9ti- ologiques senoufo (C\u00f4te-d'Ivoire). Centre national de la recherche scientifique, Paris.", "links": null }, "BIBREF6": { "ref_id": "b6", "title": "Gridline graphs: a review in two dimensions and an extension to higher dimensions", "authors": [ { "first": "Dale", "middle": [], "last": "Peterson", "suffix": "" } ], "year": 2003, "venue": "Discrete Applied Mathematics", "volume": "126", "issue": "", "pages": "223--239", "other_ids": {}, "num": null, "urls": [], "raw_text": "Dale Peterson. 2003. Gridline graphs: a review in two dimensions and an extension to higher dimensions. Discrete Applied Mathematics, 126:223-239.", "links": null }, "BIBREF7": { "ref_id": "b7", "title": "Towards robust crosslinguistic comparisons of phonological networks", "authors": [ { "first": "Philippa", "middle": [], "last": "Shoemark", "suffix": "" }, { "first": "Sharon", "middle": [], "last": "Goldwater", "suffix": "" }, { "first": "James", "middle": [], "last": "Kirby", "suffix": "" }, { "first": "Rik", "middle": [], "last": "Sarkar", "suffix": "" } ], "year": 2016, "venue": "Proceedings of the 14th ACL SIGMORPHON Workshop on Computational Research in Phonetics, Phonology, and Morphology", "volume": "", "issue": "", "pages": "110--120", "other_ids": { "DOI": [ "10.18653/v1/w16-2018" ] }, "num": null, "urls": [], "raw_text": "Philippa Shoemark, Sharon Goldwater, James Kirby, and Rik Sarkar. 2016. Towards robust cross- linguistic comparisons of phonological networks. In Proceedings of the 14th ACL SIGMORPHON Workshop on Computational Research in Phonetics, Phonology, and Morphology, pages 110-120.", "links": null }, "BIBREF8": { "ref_id": "b8", "title": "Community structure in the phonological network", "authors": [ { "first": "S Q", "middle": [], "last": "Cynthia", "suffix": "" }, { "first": "", "middle": [], "last": "Siew", "suffix": "" } ], "year": 2013, "venue": "Frontiers in Psychology", "volume": "4", "issue": "", "pages": "", "other_ids": { "DOI": [ "10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00553" ] }, "num": null, "urls": [], "raw_text": "Cynthia S Q Siew. 2013. Community structure in the phonological network. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:553.", "links": null }, "BIBREF9": { "ref_id": "b9", "title": "Investigating the influence of inverse preferential attachment on network development", "authors": [ { "first": "S Q", "middle": [], "last": "Cynthia", "suffix": "" }, { "first": "Michael", "middle": [ "S" ], "last": "Siew", "suffix": "" }, { "first": "", "middle": [], "last": "Vitevitch", "suffix": "" } ], "year": 2020, "venue": "Entropy", "volume": "22", "issue": "9", "pages": "", "other_ids": {}, "num": null, "urls": [], "raw_text": "Cynthia S Q Siew and Michael S Vitevitch. 2020. Investigating the influence of inverse preferential attachment on network development. Entropy, 22(9):1029.", "links": null }, "BIBREF10": { "ref_id": "b10", "title": "Patterns in the English language: Phonological networks, percolation and assembly models", "authors": [ { "first": "Massimo", "middle": [], "last": "Stella", "suffix": "" }, { "first": "Markus", "middle": [], "last": "Brede", "suffix": "" } ], "year": 2015, "venue": "Journal of Statistical Mechanics: Theory and Experiment", "volume": "5", "issue": "", "pages": "", "other_ids": { "DOI": [ "10.1088/1742-5468/2015/05/p05006" ] }, "num": null, "urls": [], "raw_text": "Massimo Stella and Markus Brede. 2015. Patterns in the English language: Phonological networks, per- colation and assembly models. Journal of Statistical Mechanics: Theory and Experiment, 5:P05006.", "links": null }, "BIBREF11": { "ref_id": "b11", "title": "Introduction to Graph Theory", "authors": [ { "first": "J", "middle": [], "last": "Richard", "suffix": "" }, { "first": "", "middle": [], "last": "Trudeau", "suffix": "" } ], "year": 1993, "venue": "", "volume": "", "issue": "", "pages": "", "other_ids": {}, "num": null, "urls": [], "raw_text": "Richard J Trudeau. 1993. Introduction to Graph The- ory. Dover, New York, NY.", "links": null }, "BIBREF12": { "ref_id": "b12", "title": "What governs a language's lexicon? determining the organizing principles of phonological neighbourhood networks", "authors": [ { "first": "Rory", "middle": [], "last": "Turnbull", "suffix": "" }, { "first": "Sharon", "middle": [], "last": "Peperkamp", "suffix": "" } ], "year": 2017, "venue": "Complex Networks & Their Applications V", "volume": "693", "issue": "", "pages": "83--94", "other_ids": {}, "num": null, "urls": [], "raw_text": "Rory Turnbull and Sharon Peperkamp. 2017. What governs a language's lexicon? determining the or- ganizing principles of phonological neighbourhood networks. In Hocine Cherifi, Sabrina Gaito, Walter Quattrociocchi, and Alessandra Sala, editors, Com- plex Networks & Their Applications V, volume 693 of Studies in Computational Intelligence, pages 83- 94. Springer, Cham, Switzerland.", "links": null }, "BIBREF13": { "ref_id": "b13", "title": "What can graph theory tell us about word learning and lexical retrieval?", "authors": [ { "first": "S", "middle": [], "last": "Michael", "suffix": "" }, { "first": "", "middle": [], "last": "Vitevitch", "suffix": "" } ], "year": 2008, "venue": "Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research", "volume": "51", "issue": "", "pages": "408--422", "other_ids": { "DOI": [ "10.1044/1092-4388(2008/030)" ] }, "num": null, "urls": [], "raw_text": "Michael S Vitevitch. 2008. What can graph theory tell us about word learning and lexical retrieval? Jour- nal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51:408-422.", "links": null } }, "ref_entries": { "FIGREF0": { "type_str": "figure", "uris": null, "text": "Example phonological neighbourhood network centred around the English word plan. Note that some neighbours of a word are neighbours of each other. Adapted fromTurnbull and Peperkamp (2017).", "num": null }, "FIGREF1": { "type_str": "figure", "uris": null, "text": "Three graphs. H is an induced subgraph of G formed through the removal of vertex c and its incident edges. J is also a subgraph of G, but it is not an induced subgraph due to the fact that the edge between vertices a and d is missing.diamondC The diamond graph and C 5 . Star graphs S 3 , S 4 , and S 6 . Graphs G and H and the Cartesian product G H.", "num": null }, "FIGREF2": { "type_str": "figure", "uris": null, "text": "The complete graph K 2 and Cartesian exponent K 3 2 , i.e. K 2 K 2 K 2 . The hyperlexicon H(2, {1, 2, 3}). Here the alphabet is defined as {0, 1} but any set of two symbols is possible. Edges between layers (i.e. phoneme additions/deletions) are drawn in grey; edges within layers (i.e. phoneme substitutions) are drawn in black.", "num": null }, "FIGREF3": { "type_str": "figure", "uris": null, "text": "The phonological neighbourhood network (in black) of the lexicon 1, 00, 10, and 110, depicted as a subgraph of the hyperlexicon H(2, {1, 2, 3}) (in grey).", "num": null }, "FIGREF4": { "type_str": "figure", "uris": null, "text": "C 5 as two induced subgraphs of a hyperlexicon.", "num": null }, "FIGREF5": { "type_str": "figure", "uris": null, "text": "S 5 as an induced subgraph of a hyperlexicon with max(L) = 4.", "num": null } } } }