|
{ |
|
"paper_id": "Y02-1012", |
|
"header": { |
|
"generated_with": "S2ORC 1.0.0", |
|
"date_generated": "2023-01-19T13:43:44.378509Z" |
|
}, |
|
"title": "Small Clauses and Default Case", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Youngjun", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Jang", |
|
"suffix": "", |
|
"affiliation": { |
|
"laboratory": "", |
|
"institution": "Ang University", |
|
"location": { |
|
"addrLine": "221 Hukseok-dong, Dongjak-ku", |
|
"postCode": "156-756", |
|
"settlement": "Seoul", |
|
"country": "KOREA" |
|
} |
|
}, |
|
"email": "yjang@cau.ac.kr" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": "", |
|
"venue": null, |
|
"identifiers": {}, |
|
"abstract": "This paper compares secondary predication constructions such as small clause complements, resultatives, and depictives in English and Korean. It argues that these two typologically different languages employ different modes of satisfying the Case Filter with regard to the Case of the subjects of small clauses. More specifically, it is argued that the subject of a small clause in English is Accusative Case-marked by the higher governing verb, while that in Korean is Nominative Case-marked by default.", |
|
"pdf_parse": { |
|
"paper_id": "Y02-1012", |
|
"_pdf_hash": "", |
|
"abstract": [ |
|
{ |
|
"text": "This paper compares secondary predication constructions such as small clause complements, resultatives, and depictives in English and Korean. It argues that these two typologically different languages employ different modes of satisfying the Case Filter with regard to the Case of the subjects of small clauses. More specifically, it is argued that the subject of a small clause in English is Accusative Case-marked by the higher governing verb, while that in Korean is Nominative Case-marked by default.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Abstract", |
|
"sec_num": null |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"body_text": [ |
|
{ |
|
"text": "The purpose of this paper is to compare secondary predication constructions such as small clause complements, resultatives, and depictives in English and Korean and argue that these two typologically different languages employ different modes of satisfying the Case Filter (Chomsky 1981 ) in case of the subjects of small clauses. More specifically, we argue that the subject of a small clause in English is .Accusative Case-marked by the higher governing verb, while that in Korean is Nominative Case-marked by default.", |
|
"cite_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 273, |
|
"end": 286, |
|
"text": "(Chomsky 1981", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF6" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Introduction", |
|
"sec_num": "1" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "It is generally assumed that the subject of the secondary predication has its Case assigned/checked by the matrix verb in English. Consider the following examples:", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "(1) a. I consider Here we assume a verbless complement to be a small clause (see Williams 1980 , Rothstein 1992 . Note in examples given in (1) that the subjects of these small clauses are all Case-marked by the governing verb of the matrix clause. That is, in (la), him is Accusative Case-marked by the matrix verb consider. In (lb-c), the subjects the tulips and their Nikes are each assumed to be Case-marked by the matrix verbs watered and ran, respectively. In (1d), the bone is assumed to be Case-marked by the transitive-like gnawed (see Kim and Maling 1997, among many others) .", |
|
"cite_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 76, |
|
"end": 94, |
|
"text": "(see Williams 1980", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 95, |
|
"end": 111, |
|
"text": ", Rothstein 1992", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF17" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 545, |
|
"end": 584, |
|
"text": "Kim and Maling 1997, among many others)", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "Various mechanisms are proposed to account for the Case properties of these subjects of secondary predicates. For example, Stowell (1981) argues that the matrix verb \"governs\" into the subject of small clauses. Thus in (la), consider \"governs\" into the bracketed category so that him is assigned/checked off its accusative Case. Bowers (1993 Bowers ( , 1997 Bowers ( , 2001 assumes that the subjects of small clauses are posited in a Case position. In particular, he assumes that transitive resultatives are control constructions in which the subjects of the resultative predicates are in [Spec, VP] and that intransitive resultatives are raising constructions in which the subjects of the resultative predicates are raised from [Spec, PrP] to [Spec, VP] . In case of intransitive resultatives, the subject of the secondary predicate, namely their Nikes, is raised from [Spec, PRP] to [Spec, VP] . Bowers (1997: 45) argues that the strong [Acc] case-marked subject of the resultative PrP is forced to move overtly to [Spec, VP] to check case features.' It is important for our purposes here to note that in both the transitive and intransitive structures, given in (2) and 3respectively, the subject of the (so-called) predicate phrase PrP is in [Spec, VP] to which Accusative Case is assigned by the higher governing verb of the matrix clause. More examples of intransitive resultatives are provided below:", |
|
"cite_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 123, |
|
"end": 137, |
|
"text": "Stowell (1981)", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF19" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 329, |
|
"end": 341, |
|
"text": "Bowers (1993", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 342, |
|
"end": 357, |
|
"text": "Bowers ( , 1997", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF3" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 358, |
|
"end": 373, |
|
"text": "Bowers ( , 2001", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF4" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 589, |
|
"end": 599, |
|
"text": "[Spec, VP]", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 729, |
|
"end": 740, |
|
"text": "[Spec, PrP]", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 744, |
|
"end": 754, |
|
"text": "[Spec, VP]", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 870, |
|
"end": 881, |
|
"text": "[Spec, PRP]", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 885, |
|
"end": 895, |
|
"text": "[Spec, VP]", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 898, |
|
"end": 915, |
|
"text": "Bowers (1997: 45)", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 1246, |
|
"end": 1256, |
|
"text": "[Spec, VP]", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "(2) a. The kids laughed themselves into frenzy. b. He sneezed his handkerchief completely soggy.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "c. The tenors sang themselves hoarse.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "In all of the examples above, the subjects of the secondary predicates are Accusative Case-marked by the matrix verbs, which are \"intrinsically\" intransitives. Kim and Maling (1997) argue that the matrix intransitive verbs of the intransitive resultative constructions undergo so-called \"Resultative Formation,\" whereby the intransitive verbs change to transitive verbs so that they can assign/check Case. Consider their structures:", |
|
"cite_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 160, |
|
"end": 181, |
|
"text": "Kim and Maling (1997)", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF13" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "I In general, Accusative Case in English is assumed to be weak, so that it can be checked off at LF. What Bowers intend to mean by \"strong [Acc]\" seems to be that the subject of the secondary predicate in English must be \"overtly\" Case-marked by matrix verb governing it. c. The professor lectured *(to) the class.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "In the examples in (3), the matrix verbs are claimed to undergo so-called \"Resultative Formation.\"", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "Thus the prepositions are not allowed in these examples, while in the ordinary intransitive usage in ( A process like \"Resultative Formation\" proposed in Kim and Maling (1997) seems to be needed, anyhow, to account for why the prepositions in the examples given in (5) are not allowed and, equally importantly, to explain why the \"fake\" reflexive objects are obligatory in the examples in 5below:", |
|
"cite_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 154, |
|
"end": 175, |
|
"text": "Kim and Maling (1997)", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF13" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"ref_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 101, |
|
"end": 102, |
|
"text": "(", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "(5) a. Joggers often run *(themselves) sick b. The kids laughed *(themselves) into a frenzy.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "c. The tenors sang *(themselves) hoarse.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "In the examples in (5), the reflexive objects should not be omitted. Otherwise, the Case (feature) of the matrix verbs may not be checked/saturated, after it is transformed into a transitive verb.3.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "In all of these analyses, one common feature is that Case Filter holds of the subjects of the secondary predicates in English and that Case Filter is satisfied by the \"transitive-like\" properties of 2 It seems that in English semantic transitivity might be expressed in terms of syntactic transitivity. That is, resultative itself is a semantic transitive and this transitivity seems to be expressed by syntactic \"transitivization\" of the intransitive verbs. However, it does not necessarily hold cross-linguistically. See Section 4 of this paper for the case of Korean in which we argue that Korean intransitive resultatives do not under \"Resultative Formation\" in the sense of Kim and Maling (1997) .", |
|
"cite_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 679, |
|
"end": 700, |
|
"text": "Kim and Maling (1997)", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF13" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "3 Rothstein (1992:157) argues that case consideration cannot explain the obligatoriness of the pleonastic in examples like .(i) below, since the assignment of accusative Case by a potentially Case-assigning verb is not obligatory:", |
|
"cite_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 2, |
|
"end": 22, |
|
"text": "Rothstein (1992:157)", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "I consider [*(it) obvious that they had to leave]", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "We do not entirely agree with her, instead adopting the minimalist assumption that the Case feature of a functional head/lexical head must be checked off. Another possibility is that the so called Extended Projection Principle (EPP) is violated in (i) if the pleonastic it is omitted. the matrix verbs. In sum, the subjects of the secondary predicates in English are Case-marked and the case of these subjects is assigned/checked by the matrix verbs governing the subjects. Bowers's (1997 Bowers's ( , 2001 ) raising analysis of intransitive resultatives or Kim and Maling's (1997) \"Resultative Formation\" may not be extended over to Korean data. In examples in 7, the subjects are all Nominative Case-marked. This is strikingly different from the intransitive resultatives in English.", |
|
"cite_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 474, |
|
"end": 488, |
|
"text": "Bowers's (1997", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF3" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 489, |
|
"end": 506, |
|
"text": "Bowers's ( , 2001", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF4" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 558, |
|
"end": 581, |
|
"text": "Kim and Maling's (1997)", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF13" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "Suppose that Bowers's (1997) raising analysis or Kim and Maling's (1997) Resultative", |
|
"cite_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 13, |
|
"end": 28, |
|
"text": "Bowers's (1997)", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF3" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 49, |
|
"end": 72, |
|
"text": "Kim and Maling's (1997)", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF13" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "Formation analysis are correct in that the subject of the intransitive resultative predicate discussed so far must be in a Case position. That is, suppose that it is in [Spec, VP] in Bowers (1997 Bowers ( , 2001 or in object position in Kim and Maling (1997) . Suppose further that this analysis is intended to be applied cross-linguistically. Then it is incorrectly predicted that in (9) the subjects of the resultative predicates should be Accusative Case-marked and not Nominative Case-marked.", |
|
"cite_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 169, |
|
"end": 179, |
|
"text": "[Spec, VP]", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 183, |
|
"end": 195, |
|
"text": "Bowers (1997", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF3" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 196, |
|
"end": 211, |
|
"text": "Bowers ( , 2001", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF4" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 237, |
|
"end": 258, |
|
"text": "Kim and Maling (1997)", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF13" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "If the Case Filter (Chomsky 1981 ) is a universal condition and Korean is not an eception to its application, then we need to explain how the examples in (9) would be compatible with the Case Filter. In the next subsection we are concerned with this issue.", |
|
"cite_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 19, |
|
"end": 32, |
|
"text": "(Chomsky 1981", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF6" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Case Filter and English Small Clauses", |
|
"sec_num": "2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "Given that the Case Filter is a universal condition, Korean data, particulary those in (9) seem to pose a potential problem. If the matrix verbs undergo \"Resultative Formation\" or if they are transitive verbs, then the subjects of the secondary predicates would be Accusative Case-marked. This is not the case, however.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "A careful examination, however, reveals that it is only an apparent one. It is highly conceivable that languages may differ in allowing default Case strategy. Thus English and Korean may be parameterized with regard to the default Case strategy: English does not allow default Case strategy, while Korean does allow it. In the case of secondary predication, English employs \"Raising to Case position\" (see Bowers 1993 Bowers , 1997 Bowers , 2001 or \"Resultative Formation\"(see Kim and Maling 1997) to satisfy Case Filter. Korean does not employ these apparatuses since it allows default strategy. In the next section we provide evidence for the claim that we need to admit default Case strategy in Korean. -1u1.4 There is a consensus on the assumption that structural Accusative Case in Korean is assigned by the verb to its object in transitive sentences, just like in English. Strikingly enough, however, some objects are not Accusative Case-marked but Nominative Case-marked. This is illustrated in (9) below:", |
|
"cite_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 406, |
|
"end": 417, |
|
"text": "Bowers 1993", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 418, |
|
"end": 431, |
|
"text": "Bowers , 1997", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF3" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 432, |
|
"end": 445, |
|
"text": "Bowers , 2001", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF4" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 477, |
|
"end": 497, |
|
"text": "Kim and Maling 1997)", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF13" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 706, |
|
"end": 712, |
|
"text": "-1u1.4", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "EQUATION", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 0, |
|
"end": 8, |
|
"text": "EQUATION", |
|
"ref_id": "EQREF", |
|
"raw_str": "(9) John-i Mary-ka coh-ta.", |
|
"eq_num": "(Adjective)" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "John-Nom Mary-Nom fond-Dec 'John is fond of Mary/John likes Mary.' (10) John-i chinkwu-ka iss-ta.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "John-Nom friend-Nom be-Dec 'John has friends.'", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "In (9) the theme argument Mary is Nominative Case-marked, and in (10) the theme argument chinkwu is Nominative Case-marked. This is different from their English counterparts, given in the translations.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "Y.-S. Kang (1986) Accusative Case is assigned to John in (10), since the predicate is [-stative], according to him.", |
|
"cite_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 6, |
|
"end": 17, |
|
"text": "Kang (1986)", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF9" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "4 Nominative Case marker is -i (as in John-i) if the NP ends with a consonant and Accusative Case marker is -ul (as in Mary-lul) if the NP ends with a vowel. That is the choice between the Nominative markers -ka and -i and between the Accusative markers -ul and -lul is phonologically conditioned. 5 Kang (1986) points out that there is no positive evidence that (AGR in) INFL assigns nominative Case in Korean, because Korean doesn't have AGR. He indicates that INFL in Korean doesn't have any independent properties as a head of S, and concludes that the assumption that the nominative Case is assigned by INFL in Korean is unmotivated. We will not discuss in detail the Case assigning mechanism of Korean in this paper. Readers are referred to Kang (1986) and Kang (1988 Nevertheless, the subject caki of the infinitive complement occurs with the Nominative Case marker -ka. To account for the occurrence of the Nominative Case marker on this subject of the infinitival clause, Kim (1991) argues that we need to posit default Nominative Case marking.", |
|
"cite_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 38, |
|
"end": 45, |
|
"text": "John-i)", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 119, |
|
"end": 128, |
|
"text": "Mary-lul)", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 300, |
|
"end": 311, |
|
"text": "Kang (1986)", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF9" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 747, |
|
"end": 758, |
|
"text": "Kang (1986)", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF9" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 763, |
|
"end": 773, |
|
"text": "Kang (1988", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF10" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 981, |
|
"end": 991, |
|
"text": "Kim (1991)", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF14" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "There are other pieces of supporting evidence for the claim that Korean allows default Nominative Case for caseless NPs. Throughout the paper, we have assumed that English employs a special apparatus to satisfy the Case Filter. For example, \"Resultative Formation\" (Kim and Maling 1997) or Raising to [Spec, VP] (Bowers 1997 ) is needed to satisfy the Case Filter in secondary predication. On the other hand, the Case Filter is satisfied by default Case assigning strategy in Korean.", |
|
"cite_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 265, |
|
"end": 286, |
|
"text": "(Kim and Maling 1997)", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF13" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 301, |
|
"end": 311, |
|
"text": "[Spec, VP]", |
|
"ref_id": null |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 312, |
|
"end": 324, |
|
"text": "(Bowers 1997", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF3" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "Consider the following data:", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "(13) John-un Mary-ka coh-ta. AdjectiveJohn-Top Mary-Nom fond-Dec 'John is fond of Mary/John likes Mary.'", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "The sentence (13) is more readily translatable into \"John is fond of Mary\" rather than \"John likes c. John is aware *(of) the fact.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "d. John is sure *(of) his appointment.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "In the English examples in (15), it is clear that the adjectives assign theme role to their complements.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "It is also clear, however, that they do not assign Case to their complements. In order to satisfy the Case Filter, the preposition of is inserted in these particular cases.'", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "In contrast to this of-insertion strategy for satisfying the Case Filter, Korean seems to employ As is well known, the subject position of the infinitival clause in English is not a Case position (see footnote 10). Given the Case Filter, however, the subject him must be assigned any Case. Since", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "English does not allow default Case strategy, him must be assigned Case by some appropriate Caseassigning head. The preposition-like complementizer for is thus employed only for Case theoretic reasons. Once again, an argument NP must be assigned Case in some way or other.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "In sum, English does not allow default Case so that it employs of-insertion or prepositional complementizer for in cases where there is no source of Case. On the other hand, Korean allows default Nominative Case so that it does not need any special apparatus for satisfying the Case Filter. In this paper we argued that English and Korean differ from each other in satisfying the Case Filter.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "More specifically, we argued that the subject of a small clause in English is Accusative Case-marked by the matrix verb governing the subject, while that in Korean is assigned default Nominative Case.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "In Section 2, we discussed the general case properties of the subjects of secondary predicates in English, and showed that the subjects of small clauses should be Accusative Case-marked either by raising to an appropriate Case position (Bowers 1997 (Bowers , 2001 or by changing the matrix verb into a \"transitive\" one (Kim and Maling 1997) . By contrast, we showed that the subjects of small clauses in Korean are either Accusative Case-marked or Nominative Case-marked according to the types of the matrix verbs. That is, if the matrix verb governing the subject of a small clause is transitive, then the subject is Accusative Case-marked. If the matrix verb governing the subject of a small clause is intransitive, on the other hand, then the subject is Nominative Case-marked. In this section, we 10 D=Dative Case, A=Accusative Case, and N=Nominative Case 132", |
|
"cite_spans": [ |
|
{ |
|
"start": 236, |
|
"end": 248, |
|
"text": "(Bowers 1997", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF3" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 249, |
|
"end": 263, |
|
"text": "(Bowers , 2001", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF4" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"start": 319, |
|
"end": 340, |
|
"text": "(Kim and Maling 1997)", |
|
"ref_id": "BIBREF13" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "proposed that Korean employs default Case strategy in order to satisfy the Case Filter on the subject of a small clause when there is no source of case assignment. In Section 4, we discussed the default Case strategy from a more broad perspective. We argued that English allows of-insertion and/or a preposition-like complementizer for in order to mark Case on caseless nouns, while Korean allows default Nominative Case for caseless nouns. Default Case in other languages is also discussed in this section.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "Default Nominative Case", |
|
"sec_num": "3.2" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "The English glossary for sipheha-n-ta in the original sentence was \"want\". However, we changed it to \"hope\" to emphasize the control property of the given verb. 7 We will not discuss the status of null Case ofPRO. See Martin (1999) for null Case assignment on PRO.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "", |
|
"sec_num": null |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "InChomsky (1986) it is argued that the preposition of is a realization of the inherent case borne by the complement of the theta-role assigning head. Admitting this, we still need to explain why the preposition of is inserted because other inherent case does not appear in some cases, as in English indirect-direct object order.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "", |
|
"sec_num": null |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "The subject position of control infmitival constructions is assumed to be assigned Null Case. SeeMartin (1999Martin ( , 2001 andChomsky (1995).", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "", |
|
"sec_num": null |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"text": "Williams, Edwin. 1985. PRO and subject of NP. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 3: 297-315.", |
|
"cite_spans": [], |
|
"ref_spans": [], |
|
"eq_spans": [], |
|
"section": "", |
|
"sec_num": null |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"back_matter": [], |
|
"bib_entries": { |
|
"BIBREF1": { |
|
"ref_id": "b1", |
|
"title": "Acc go-Comp do-Past-Dec 'John made Mary", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "John-Top", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Mary", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": null, |
|
"venue": "", |
|
"volume": "", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "John-Top Mary-Acc go-Comp do-Past-Dec 'John made Mary go.'", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF2": { |
|
"ref_id": "b2", |
|
"title": "There seems to be no controversy on this point. The English counterpart of this example, given in the translation, suggests that Mary is Accusative Case-References Bowers, John. 1993. The syntax of predication", |
|
"authors": [], |
|
"year": null, |
|
"venue": "Linguistic Inquiry", |
|
"volume": "24", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "591--656", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "In (16) the matrix verb hayssta, which is a transitive (or causative) verb, assigns Accusative Case to the subject of the small clause, namely Mary. There seems to be no controversy on this point. The English counterpart of this example, given in the translation, suggests that Mary is Accusative Case- References Bowers, John. 1993. The syntax of predication. Linguistic Inquiry 24: 591-656.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF3": { |
|
"ref_id": "b3", |
|
"title": "A binary analysis of resultatives", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "John", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Bowers", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 1997, |
|
"venue": "Texas Linguistic Forum", |
|
"volume": "38", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "48--58", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "Bowers, John. 1997. A binary analysis of resultatives. In R. Blight and M. Moosally (eds.) Texas Linguistic Forum 38: 48-58.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF4": { |
|
"ref_id": "b4", |
|
"title": "The Handbook of Contemporary Syntactic Theory", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "John", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Bowers", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 2001, |
|
"venue": "", |
|
"volume": "", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "299--333", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "Bowers, John. 2001. Predication. In M. Baltin and C. Collins (eds.) The Handbook of Contemporary Syntactic Theory. Cambridge, Mass.: Blackwell. Pp. 299-333.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF5": { |
|
"ref_id": "b5", |
|
"title": "The argument structure and syntactic structure of resultatives", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Jill", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Carrier", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Janet", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Randall", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 1992, |
|
"venue": "Linguistic Inquiry", |
|
"volume": "23", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "173--234", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "Carrier, Jill and Janet Randall. 1992. The argument structure and syntactic structure of resultatives. Linguistic Inquiry 23: 173-234.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF6": { |
|
"ref_id": "b6", |
|
"title": "Lectures on Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Noam", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Chomsky", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 1981, |
|
"venue": "", |
|
"volume": "", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "Chomsky, Noam. 1981. Lectures on Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF7": { |
|
"ref_id": "b7", |
|
"title": "Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin, and Use", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Noam", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Chomsky", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 1986, |
|
"venue": "", |
|
"volume": "", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "Chomsky, Noam. 1986. Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin, and Use. New York: Praeger.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF8": { |
|
"ref_id": "b8", |
|
"title": "On the so-called adjunct predicates in Korean", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Youngjun", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Jong", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 1997, |
|
"venue": "Texas Linguistic Form", |
|
"volume": "38", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "149--160", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "Jong, Youngjun. 1997. On the so-called adjunct predicates in Korean. In R. Blight and M. Moosally (eds.) Texas Linguistic Form 38: 149-160.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF9": { |
|
"ref_id": "b9", |
|
"title": "Korean syntax and universal grammar. Doctoral dissertation", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Young-Se", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Kang", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 1986, |
|
"venue": "", |
|
"volume": "", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "Kang, Young-Se. 1986. Korean syntax and universal grammar. Doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, Cambridge.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF10": { |
|
"ref_id": "b10", |
|
"title": "Topics in Korean syntax. Doctoral dissertation", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Myung-Yoon", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Kang", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 1988, |
|
"venue": "", |
|
"volume": "", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "Kang, Myung-Yoon. 1988. Topics in Korean syntax. Doctoral dissertation, MIT, Cambridge.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF11": { |
|
"ref_id": "b11", |
|
"title": "The -key constructions in Korean: Predicate Head or Complementizer", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Eun-Kyung", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Kang", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 2001, |
|
"venue": "9th International Symposium on Korean Linguistics", |
|
"volume": "", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "Kang, Eun-Kyung. 2001. The -key constructions in Korean: Predicate Head or Complementizer. Paper presented at the 9th International Symposium on Korean Linguistics, Harvard University.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF12": { |
|
"ref_id": "b12", |
|
"title": "Constraints on the formation of Korean and English resultative constrtuctions", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Jong-Bok", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Kim", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 1999, |
|
"venue": "NELS", |
|
"volume": "31", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "137--152", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "Kim, Jong-Bok. 1999. Constraints on the formation of Korean and English resultative constrtuctions. NELS 31: 137-152.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF13": { |
|
"ref_id": "b13", |
|
"title": "A crosslinguistic perspective on resultative formation", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Soowon", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Kim", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
}, |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Joan", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Maling", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 1997, |
|
"venue": "Texas Linguistic Forum", |
|
"volume": "38", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "189--204", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "Kim, Soowon and Joan Maling. 1997. A crosslinguistic perspective on resultative formation. In R. Blight and M. Moosally (eds.) Texas Linguistic Forum 38: 189-204.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF14": { |
|
"ref_id": "b14", |
|
"title": "Default nominative case in Korean", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Young-Joo", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Kim", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 1991, |
|
"venue": "Studies in Generative Grammar", |
|
"volume": "2", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "113--148", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "Kim, Young-joo. 1991. Default nominative case in Korean. Studies in Generative Grammar 2: 113-148.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF15": { |
|
"ref_id": "b15", |
|
"title": "Case, movement and raising in modem Irish", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "James", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Mcclosky", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 1985, |
|
"venue": "", |
|
"volume": "", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "McClosky, James. 1985. Case, movement and raising in modem Irish. WCCFL 4.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF16": { |
|
"ref_id": "b16", |
|
"title": "The syntactic form of predication. Doctoral dissertation", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Susan", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Rothstein", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 1983, |
|
"venue": "", |
|
"volume": "", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "Rothstein, Susan. 1983. The syntactic form of predication. Doctoral dissertation, MIT, Cambridge.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF17": { |
|
"ref_id": "b17", |
|
"title": "Predication and the structure of clauses", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Susan", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Rothstein", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 1992, |
|
"venue": "Belgian Journal of Linguistics", |
|
"volume": "7", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "153--169", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "Rothstein, Susan. 1992. Predication and the structure of clauses. Belgian Journal of Linguistics 7: 153-169.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF18": { |
|
"ref_id": "b18", |
|
"title": "Structural relationships within complex predicates", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Peter", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Sells", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 1998, |
|
"venue": "Proceedings of the 1998 Biannual Meeting of the International Circle of Korean Linguistics", |
|
"volume": "", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "Sells, Peter. 1998. Structural relationships within complex predicates. Proceedings of the 1998 Biannual Meeting of the International Circle of Korean Linguistics, Hawaii.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF19": { |
|
"ref_id": "b19", |
|
"title": "Origins of phrase structure. Doctoral dissertation", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Tim", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Stowell", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 1981, |
|
"venue": "", |
|
"volume": "", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "Stowell, Tim. 1981. Origins of phrase structure. Doctoral dissertation, MIT, Cambridge.", |
|
"links": null |
|
}, |
|
"BIBREF20": { |
|
"ref_id": "b20", |
|
"title": "On complementation in Icelandic. Doctoral dissertation", |
|
"authors": [ |
|
{ |
|
"first": "Hoskuldur", |
|
"middle": [], |
|
"last": "Thriinsson", |
|
"suffix": "" |
|
} |
|
], |
|
"year": 1979, |
|
"venue": "", |
|
"volume": "", |
|
"issue": "", |
|
"pages": "", |
|
"other_ids": {}, |
|
"num": null, |
|
"urls": [], |
|
"raw_text": "Thriinsson, Hoskuldur. 1979. On complementation in Icelandic. Doctoral dissertation, Harvard University,", |
|
"links": null |
|
} |
|
}, |
|
"ref_entries": { |
|
"FIGREF0": { |
|
"num": null, |
|
"type_str": "figure", |
|
"text": "[him honest] (complement small clause) b. The gardener watered [the tulips flat] (transitive resultative) c. The joggers ran [their Nikes threadbare] (intransitive resultative) d. The lion gnawed (*on) [the bone raw] (object depictive) (la) contains a complement small clause, (lb-c) contain resultative small clauses, and (1d) contains a depictive small clause.", |
|
"uris": null |
|
}, |
|
"FIGREF1": { |
|
"num": null, |
|
"type_str": "figure", |
|
"text": "lion gnawed (*on) [the bone raw] b. The winemakers stomped (*on) the grapes flat. c. The professor lectured (*to) the class into a stupor. (4) a. The lion gnawed *(on) the bone b. The winemakers stomped *(on) the grapes.", |
|
"uris": null |
|
}, |
|
"FIGREF2": { |
|
"num": null, |
|
"type_str": "figure", |
|
"text": "Mary.\" The adjective coh-ta 'fond' assigns theme role to its sister, but it does not assign Case to its sister. This is just like the English counterpart shown in(14): (14) a. John is fond *(of) Mary. b. John is proud *(of) his son.", |
|
"uris": null |
|
}, |
|
"FIGREF3": { |
|
"num": null, |
|
"type_str": "figure", |
|
"text": "Default Case in Other Languages Icelandic also allows Nominative NPs to occur in the subject position of certain infinitival clauses. First of all, consider the examples in (21), cited from Thr insson (1979: 299, 301):b0 (21) a. Maria skipaOi [honum a8 vera goaur/goaum/*goaan] Mary ordered him(D) to be good(N / D/*A) b. Maria bad [pa aa vera goOir/goba/gobum] Mary asked them(A) to be good(N/A/*D) (22) a. Eg bad [hann aa fara einn/einani*einum pangab] I asked him(A) to go alone(N/A *D) there b. Eg skipabi [henni aa fara ein/einni/eina pangaO] I ordered her(D) to go alone(N/D/*A) there c. Mer viraist [Anna vera veik] me-Dat seems Anna-Nom to-be sick The examples in (21) are object-controlled infinitival constructions. In (21a), there is case agreement between predicative adjectives and their PRO subject. In (21b), subject-oriented adjuncts agree in case with their PRO type subjects controlled by the object of matrix verbs. The basic generalization about \"object-controlled Equi\" sentences like (21a,b) is that the predicative adjectives and subject-oriented adjuncts can either agree in case with the matrix controller (=the matrix object) or show up in the nominative, suggesting a covert nominative marking of PRO. In certain raising constructions like (22c), a nominative lexical NP overtly shows up in the subject position of infinitival complements. 5 Concluding Remarks", |
|
"uris": null |
|
}, |
|
"TABREF1": { |
|
"html": null, |
|
"type_str": "table", |
|
"content": "<table/>", |
|
"text": "3 Small Clauses in KoreanIn this section we are concerned with the two Case forms of the subjects of small clauses in Korean. In Section 3.1, it is shown that the subject of a small clause is Accusative Case-marked if the matrix verb is transitive, while that is Nominative Case-marked if the matrix verb is intransitive. In Section 3.2, we argue that the subject of a small clause in Korean is Nominative Case-marked by default if there is no case assigned (to the subject of the small clause) by the matrix verb.", |
|
"num": null |
|
}, |
|
"TABREF2": { |
|
"html": null, |
|
"type_str": "table", |
|
"content": "<table><tr><td colspan=\"3\">This is illustrated in (6) below:</td><td/></tr><tr><td>(6)</td><td>a. Robin-i</td><td>[soy-luU*ka</td><td colspan=\"2\">ttukep-key] takkwu-ess-ta. (resultative)</td></tr><tr><td/><td colspan=\"4\">Robin-Nom metal-Acc/*Nom hot-Comp heat-Past-Dec</td></tr><tr><td/><td colspan=\"2\">Robin heated the metal hot.'</td><td/></tr><tr><td/><td colspan=\"2\">b. na-nun [Robin-u1/*-i</td><td colspan=\"2\">cohci ahn-key] yeki-n-ta. (complement)</td></tr><tr><td/><td colspan=\"4\">I-Top Robin-Acc/*Nom not good-Comp consider-Prs-Dec</td></tr><tr><td/><td colspan=\"3\">I consider Robin not good.'</td></tr><tr><td/><td colspan=\"3\">c. Robin-un umsik-u1/*-i cca-key</td><td>mek-nun-ta (depictive)</td></tr><tr><td/><td colspan=\"4\">Robin-Top food-Acc/*Nom salty-Comp eat-Prs-Ind</td></tr><tr><td/><td colspan=\"2\">Robin eats food salty.'</td><td/></tr><tr><td colspan=\"2\">(7) a. Robin-i</td><td colspan=\"3\">[payldcop-Mul ppaci-key] wus-ess-ta (intransitive resultative)</td></tr><tr><td/><td colspan=\"4\">Robin-Nom belly-Nom/*Acc come.out-key laugh-Pst-Ind</td></tr><tr><td/><td colspan=\"2\">Robin laughed his belly out.'</td><td/></tr></table>", |
|
"text": "The subjects of the secondary predicates in Korean are invariably Accusative Case-marked if the matrix verbs are transitive verbs.In the examples in (6), the subjects of the secondary predicates are all Accusative Case-marked and not Nominative Case-marked. On the other hand, the subjects of the secondary predicate are invariably Nominative Case-marked if the matrix verbs are intransitive. Consider the following: Robin waited (for Mary) (so long) that his eyes almost came out.' c.Robin-un [kwutwu-ka/nul talh-key] talli-ess-ta.", |
|
"num": null |
|
}, |
|
"TABREF5": { |
|
"html": null, |
|
"type_str": "table", |
|
"content": "<table><tr><td colspan=\"3\">Y.-J. Kim (1991:135) provides several more arguments for default Nominative Case in</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"3\">Korean. According to her, a lexical NP can appear in the subject position of infinitival control</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"3\">constructions. Let us consider the following example, slightly changed from her sentence:</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"3\">(12) a. Inho-ka [s PRO/caki-ka ka]-ko] sipheha-n-ta.6</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"3\">Inho-Nom PRO/self-Nom go-comp hope-Pres-Dec</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"2\">'Inho hopes PRO/self to go.'</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"2\">b. Inho-ka [s, [s PRO/caki-ka</td><td>ku kes-ul ha]-lyeko] ayssu-ess-ta.</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"2\">Inho-Nom PRO/self-Nom</td><td>that thing-Acc do-Comp endeavor-Past-Dec</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"3\">'Inho endeavored PRO/self do it.'</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"3\">Korean infinitival complement constructions like (12) have \"obligatory control\"property. That is, the</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"2\">following sentence is ungrammatical:</td></tr><tr><td>(12 ) *Inho-ka [s Yumi-ka</td><td colspan=\"2\">ka-ko] sipheha-n-ta.</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"3\">Inho-Nom Yumi-Nom go-Comp hope-Pres-Dec</td></tr><tr><td>'*Inho hopes Yumi to go'</td><td/></tr></table>", |
|
"text": "). because the verb in these sentences is not[-stative].M.-Y.Kang (1988:35) suggests a partial Default-Nominative Case Hypothesis. He claims that the Nominative Case-marking of the experiencer NP, namely John in (9-10), as a consequence of structural Nominative Case assignment by INFL, whereas Nominative Case-marking on the theme NP, namely Mary and chinkwu, is viewed as a result of default Nominative Case assignment.Returning to the control structure (12), the subject of the infinitival clause is PRO. As is well established, PRO must not be governed and hence is not Case-marked.' That is, the subject position of the infinitival clause in (12) is not a Case position. Therefore, the nominative Case on the reflexive subject, namely caki, cannot be assigned any Case. simply because this position is not a Case position.", |
|
"num": null |
|
}, |
|
"TABREF6": { |
|
"html": null, |
|
"type_str": "table", |
|
"content": "<table><tr><td>(17)</td><td>[ip ku-ka cinaka-tolok] John-up pilhyese-ss-ta</td></tr><tr><td/><td>he-Nom pass by-Comp John-Top stand aside-Past-Dec</td></tr><tr><td/><td>'John stood aside in order [for him to pass by].'</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"2\">The embedded clause in (17) is an infinitival clause. Hence there is no tense or agreement marker in</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"2\">this infinitival clause, as shown in (18):</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"2\">(18) *[rp ku-ka cinaka-ss-tolok] John-up pilhyese-ss-ta</td></tr><tr><td/><td>he-Nom pass by-Past-Comp John-Top stand aside-Past-Dec</td></tr><tr><td/><td>'John stood aside in order [for him to pass by].'</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"2\">situation. Compare this sentence with its English counterpart, given in (19):</td></tr><tr><td colspan=\"2\">(20) [For him to pass by], John stood aside.</td></tr></table>", |
|
"text": "default Nominative Case. In other words, in cases like (15), where the adjective coh-ta 'fond' assigns theme role to its sister but it does not assign Case, as mentioned before, the Caseless NP, namely Mary, is assigned default Nominative Case.Another case of default Nominative Case in Korean is witnessed in infmitival constructions.Before discussing infmitival constructions, consider the following small clause construction: marked by the matrix verb made. Now let . us consider the infinitival clauses in Korean. It is important to note that the subject position of infinitival clauses is not a Case position.9As in English, the infmitival clause in Korean does not carry tense markers. Hence the ungrammaticality of (18). Given that in (17) the bracketed embedded clause is infinitival, there is no source of the Nominative Case on ku, the subject of the embedded infmitival clause. If the subject ku is not Case-assigned, then the sentence would incorrectly be judged to be ungrammatical. This is, however, not the case. Thus we are led to admit that Korean allows default Nominative Case in this", |
|
"num": null |
|
} |
|
} |
|
} |
|
} |